There is a prevailing notion that humans are basically selfish. Our behaviour and actions are motivated primarily by self-interest, this view says.
Is it true?
In common with many answers to questions of this type,
the answer here is yes, and no. It is only partially true. However, it is also
predominantly wrong.
Within westernised cultures the concept of self and
individualism is well established. So well established, in fact, that the idea
that we are selfish by nature is entrenched within our cultural psyche.
There are however, at least three reasons for doubting
the universality of this notion. 1. Other cultures and spiritual traditions are
more cooperatively focused. 2. Recent research suggests an inherent cooperative
bias. 3. The idea shows little imagination on our part.
Other Cultures and Spiritual Traditions
Many indigenous and nature-based cultures show a
greater sense of cooperation than do westernised cultures. In such cultures the
self is often synonymous with us and a collective approach to
life.
Within the Zulu culture the word ubuntu
describes such an identification. Bishop Tutu defines ubuntu as "the
philosophy and belief that a person is only a person through other
people. In other words, we are human only in relation to other
humans. Our humanity is bound up in one another… This interconnectedness
is the very root of who we are."1
This
idea of the self being bound up with others is revealed also in the concept of interbeing
– a word coined by Vietnamese Buddhist monk Thich Nhat Hanh. He describes this
idea as “the many in the one and the one containing the many.” In a nod to
Descartes, he goes on to say that interbeing can be expressed as: “I am, therefore you are. You are, therefore I am. We
inter-are.”2
There
are some who claim that those who act cooperatively and fairly do so out of
self-interest. It is, they claim, in ones self-interest to act cooperatively.
This critique is nothing short of cultural colonialism – viewing indigenous
culture and understandings through the lens of colonial prejudice.
Recent
Research
Just as
Thich Nhat Hanh gives a nod to Descartes, so does Stefan Klein give a nod to
Darwin, titling his 2014 book Survival of the Nicest.3 Noting that sharing and fairness
occurs freely within children, he concludes that “If this inclination to
fairness appears at such an early age, it is highly likely that it is innate.”
Klein
further suggests, convincingly, that humans developed the social intelligence
of cooperation before abstract and language intelligence. “We humans
became first the friendliest and then the most intelligent apes,” he asserts.
Researchers
within Evolutionary Dynamics and Psychology at Harvard University published an
interesting paper in Nature in September 2012.4 In studies involving almost 2,000
people they tracked the difference in cooperative decision-making versus
self-interested decision making. The research looked at the speed of
decision-making, recognising that quick decisions are more likely to be instinctive
and/or intuitive, whereas longer, more considered decision-making tended to be
rational.
In all
cases faster decision-making was significantly more likely to show a
cooperative outcome than a self-interested outcome, and vice versa.
Instinctual
and intuitive responses are more likely to demonstrate an innate trait than are
more considered responses. Hence, it can be claimed, as the researchers here
did, that, “Our results provide convergent evidence that intuition supports
cooperation in social dilemmas, and that reflection can undermine these
cooperative impulses.”
Imagination
As suggested earlier, the notion that humans are innately
selfish is entrenched within our (westernised) cultural psyche. So much so that
we find it difficult to imagine that it may be otherwise. In our popular
literature, on the big screen, and elsewhere, we see and read of images of
stone age humans brutalising one another, invading and occupying another clans
territory. We don’t stop to question this. It has always been this way – hasn’t
it?
Albert Einstein noted that imagination is more important
than knowledge. Our ability to imagine is often the precursor to a deeper
enquiry into phenomena or problems. Imagination is certainly the inspiration
for questioning.
One person who did imagine a more cooperative inner nature
was the Tibetan monk, Chögyam Trungpa Rinpoche. In 1976 this monk brought the
Shambhala Vision to the attention of the western world. The vision included
Shambhala warriors, although the Tibetan word, pawo (translated into
English as warrior) more correctly
means one who
is brave.
Chögyam Trungpa explains that within this vision bravery
means not being afraid of oneself. He explains:5
“The Shambhala vision is the opposite
of selfishness. When we are afraid of ourselves and afraid of the seeming
threat that the world presents, then we become extremely selfish. We want to
build our own little nests, our own little cocoons, so that we can live by
ourselves in a more secure way.”
Can we do it? Can we be brave? Can we be unafraid of
ourselves? Can we imagine a cooperative innateness?
We can, and we must.
Notes:
1. Tutu,
Desmond & Tutu, Mpho: The Book of
Forgiving, William Collins, London, 2014.
2. Thich
Nhat Hanh, Being Peace, Parallax Press, Berkeley, California, 1987.
3. Stefan
Klein, Survival of the Nicest, Scribe Publications, Victoria (Australia)
and London, 2014. Note also that the phrase ‘survival of the fittest’ was
not used by Darwin, although many assume it was.
4. Rand,
Greene, and Nowak, Spontaneous giving and calculated greed, in Nature,
20 September 2012, Vol 489, pp 427-430.
5. Chögyam Trungpa Rinpoche, The Discovery of Basic Goodness, in Melvin McLeod
(ed) Mindful
Politics, Wisdom Publications, Boston, 2006, pp 98-103.
Great blogging Bruce! We are One! Be Mindful... Pause... Connect!
ReplyDelete